New Study Reveals How the Brain Says Oops! – Neuroscience News

Summary: Researchers identified a group of neurons in the frontal lobe that enables flexibility to learn new skills and the focus to develop sophisticated skills. The brain uses the same group of neurons for performance feedback in a variety of situations.

Source; Cedars Sinai

Researchers from Cedars-Sinais Center for Neural Science and Medicine and Department of Neurosurgery have uncovered how signals from a group of neurons in the brains frontal lobe simultaneously give humans the flexibility to learn new tasksand the focus to develop highly specific skills.

Their research, published today in the peer-reviewed journalScience,provides a fundamental understanding of performance monitoring, an executive function used to manage daily life.

The studys key finding is that the brain uses the same group of neurons for performance feedback in many different situationswhether a person is attempting a newtaskfor the first time or working to perfect a specific skill.

Part of the magic of the human brain is that it is so flexible, said Ueli Rutishauser, Ph.D., professor of Neurosurgery, Neurology, and Biomedical Sciences, director of the Center for Neural Science and Medicine, the Board of Governors Chair in Neurosciences and senior author of the study. We designed our study to decipher how the brain can generalizeandspecialize at the same time, both of which are critical for helping us pursue a goal.

Performancemonitoringis an internal signal, a kind of self-generated feedback, that lets a person know they have made a mistake. One example is the person who realizes they drove past an intersection where they should have turned. Another example is the person who says something in conversation and recognizes as soon as the words are out of their mouth that what they just said was inappropriate.

That Oh, shoot moment, that Oops! moment, is performance monitoring kicking in, said Zhongzheng Fu, Ph.D., a postdoctoral researcher in the Rutishauser Laboratory at Cedars-Sinai and first author of the study.

These signals help improve performance on future attempts by passing information to areas of the brain that regulate emotions, memory, planning and problem-solving. Performance monitoring also helps the brain adjust its focus by signaling how much conflict or difficulty was encountered during the task.

So an Oops! moment might prompt someone to pay closer attention the next time they chat with a friend or plan to stop at the store on the way home from work, said Fu.

To see performance monitoring in action, investigators recorded the activity of individual neurons in the medial frontal cortex of study participants. The participants wereepilepsy patientswho, as part of their treatment, had electrodes implanted in their brains to help locate the focus of their seizures. Specifically, these patients had electrodes implanted in the medial frontal cortex, a brain region known to play a central role in performance monitoring.

In the Stroop task, which pits reading against color naming, participants viewed the written name of a color, such as red, printed in ink of a different color, such as green, and were asked to name the ink color rather than the written word.

This creates conflict in the brain, Rutishauser said. You have decades of training in reading, but now your goal is to suppress that habit of reading and say the color of the ink that the word is written in instead.

In the other task, the Multi-Source Interference Task (MSIT), which involves recognizing numerals, participants saw three numerical digits on screen, two the same and the other uniquefor example, 1-2-2. The subjects task was to press the button associated with the unique numberin this case, 1resisting their tendency to press 2 because that number appears twice.

These two tasks serve as a strong test of how self-monitoring is engaged in different scenarios involving different cognitive domains, Fu said.

A structured response

As the subjects performed these tasks, the investigators noted two different types of neurons at work. Error neurons fired strongly after an error was made, while conflict neurons fired in response to the difficulty of the task the subject had just performed.

When we observed the activity of neurons in this brain area, it surprised us that most of them only become active after a decision or an action was completed. This indicates that this brain area plays a role in evaluating decisions after the fact, rather than making them.

There are two types of performance monitoring: domain general and domain specific. Domain general performance monitoring tells ussomethingwent wrong and can detect errors in any type of taskwhether someone is driving a car, navigating a social situation or playing Wordle for the first time. This allows them to perform new tasks with little instruction, something machines cannot do.

Machines can be trained to do one thing really well, Fu said. You can build a robot to flip hamburgers, but it cant adapt those skills to frying dumplings. Humans, thanks to domain general performance monitoring, can.

Domain specific performance monitoring tells the person who made the errorwhatwent wrong, detecting specific mistakesthat they missed a turn, said something inappropriate or chose the wrong letter in a puzzle. This is one way people perfect individual skills.

Surprisingly, neurons signaling domain general and domain specific information were intermingled in the medial frontal cortex.

We used to think there were portions of the brain dedicated to only domain general performance monitoring and others to only domain specific, Rutishauser said.

Our study now shows thats not the case. Weve learned that the very same group of neurons can do both domain general and domain specific performance monitoring. When youre listening to these neurons, you can read out both types of information simultaneously.

To understand how these signals are interpreted by other areas of the brain, it helps to think of the neurons as musicians in an orchestra, Rutishauser said.

If they all play at random, the listenersin this case the regions of thebrainreceiving the signalsjust hear a garbled set of notes, Rutishauser said.

But if they play an arranged composition, its possible to clearly hear the various melodies and harmonies even with so many instrumentsor performance monitoring neuronsplaying all at once.

Too much or too little of this signaling, however, can cause problems, Rutishauser said.

Overactive performance monitoring can manifest asobsessive-compulsive disorder, causing a person to check obsessively for errors that dont exist. At the other extreme is schizophrenia, where performance monitoring can be underactive to a degree that a person doesnt perceive errors or the inappropriateness of their words or actions.

We believe the mechanistic knowledge we have gained will be critical to perfecting treatments for these devastating psychiatric disorders, Rutishauser said.

The research team also included Jeffrey Chung, MD, director of the Cedars-Sinai Epilepsy Program; Assistant Professor of Neurology Chrystal Reed, MD, Ph.D.; Adam Mamelak, MD, professor of neurosurgery and director of the Functional Neurosurgery Program; Ralph Adolphs, Ph.D., professor of Psychology, Neuroscience, and Biology at the California Institute of Technology; and research associate Danielle Beam.

Author: Press OfficeSource: Cedars SinaiContact: Press Office Cedars SinaiImage: The image is in the public domain

Original Research: Closed access.The geometry of domain-general performance monitoring in the human medial frontal cortex by Zhongzheng Fu et al. Science

Abstract

The geometry of domain-general performance monitoring in the human medial frontal cortex

Controlling behavior to flexibly achieve desired goals depends on the ability to monitor ones own performance. It is unknown how performance monitoring can be both flexible, to support different tasks, and specialized, to perform each task well.

We recorded single neurons in the human medial frontal cortex while subjects performed two tasks that involve three types of cognitive conflict. Neurons encoding conflict probability, conflict, and error in one or both tasks were intermixed, forming a representational geometry that simultaneously allowed task specialization and generalization. Neurons encoding conflict retrospectively served to update internal estimates of conflict probability. Population representations of conflict were compositional.

These findings reveal how representations of evaluative signals can be both abstract and task-specific and suggest a neuronal mechanism for estimating control demand.

Read the original here:
New Study Reveals How the Brain Says Oops! - Neuroscience News

Related Posts