Category Archives: Neuroscience

Low-Calorie Diets Harm Athletes Performance and Health – Neuroscience News

Summary: Female athletes consuming only half their caloric needs for 14 days experienced significant drops in performance and muscle mass. This low energy availability also weakened their immune systems.

The harmful effects couldnt be reversed by short-term refeeding, highlighting the risks of weight loss practices in sports. The findings emphasize the need for awareness and better support for athletes.

Key Facts:

Source: University of Copenhagen

Whether selected to swim, row or run in the Olympics, or gearing up to ride in the Tour de France, achieving the right weight has been a focal point of many elite athletes for decades. It could be to look lean and mean in a swimsuit or jersey, or to qualify for a certain weight category. But there is also a belief that losing weight enhances performance.

As such, it is a widespread phenomenon among athletesespecially inendurance sportslike running, swimming, cycling and rowingto reduce theirdietary intakein the run-up to competition.

It is particularly problematic among female endurance athletes. Many athletes focus heavily on weight in their respective sports. Consequently, they tend to go into short-term, but intense periods of weight loss with the expectation of performing better, says Professor Ylva Hellsten of the University of Copenhagens Department of Nutrition, Exercise and Sports.

She and Ph.D. student Jan Sommer Jeppesen are two of the researchers behinda new studyon the effects of low energy availability amongfemale athletes.

The paper is published in the journalRedox Biology.

We know that the phenomenon of not eating enough is associated with many things that are harmful to healthincluding missed periods, compromised bone health and changes in metabolism. But there is still plenty that we dont know. As such, we investigated some of the possible consequences more closely, says Jeppesen, who is the studys lead author.

For the study, the researchers recruited twelve female triathletes, all of whom had a normal energy intake. During one part of the trial, the athletes were given enough calories for 14 days, after which their performance was tested. The same athletes also went through a 14-day period during which they consumed only about 50% of their energy needs while sticking to their normal intensive training schedule.

During the period with insufficient calories, athletes lost an average of roughly 4% of their body weight, about half of which was muscle mass. And they experienced a loss in performance:

The fourteen days of insufficient food intake reduced their performance by 7.7% in a 20-minute time trial on a bike, which is quite significant. And during a more intense short-term test, their performance slid by as much as 18%. So there is no doubt that this practice greatly impairs ones performance as an athlete, even over shorter periods of time, says Jeppesen.

In addition to sports performance, the researchers examined the effects on athletes immune function.

Among other things, we saw that insufficient energy intake was associated with increased systemic stress. The athletes had a large increase in cortisol, a stress hormone, and a dramatically increased stress level in immune cells. This suggests that there is a quite severe impact on several aspects of the immune system if one doesnt eat enough. This may potentially contribute to athletes being more exposed to illness, says Jeppesen.

The researchers hope that the results of the study will help create more awareness of the phenomenon.

Many coaches continue to pressure athletes to lose weight. For many years, it has been a part of the culture in the sports worldand remains so. We need to shed light on the phenomenon and ask critically: What are we actually doing to our athletes both physically and psychologically? says Hellsten.

Team Denmark, the Danish elite sport organization, welcomes the new research results with open arms.

It focuses on a really important topic and challenges the attitude that lighter is always better. The theory and culture remains prevalent in many sports. I experience many athletes who trim their weight in the weeks leading up to a competition, but without understanding the consequences of doing so, says Majke Jrgensen, a sports nutritionist and manager at Team Denmark.

She sees the results as useful knowledge that can support a message that Team Denmark has been trying to promote:

My experience is that elite athletes and coaches are curious, but need research that backs up any critiques of the phenomenon. Here, the fact that thetest subjectsare actual athletes is a major strength, so that the results can be transferred to the athletes and coaches that Team Denmark supports.

We will use these results to support what we are already trying to communicate, both when we sit down with athletes one-on-one, as well as during workshops and presentations in these types of contexts, says Jrgensen.

After fourteen days of low energy availability (LEA), the athletes underwent a three-day refeeding period as part of the trial, during which they were provided plenty to eat.

We had expected that the three days of enough food would restore their performanceand maybe even improve itbut there was absolutely no effect. Their performance was just as degraded as prior to the three days. This tells us that the negative effects cannot be reversed by quickly replenishing energy stores, which is a strategy used by many athletes, says Jeppesen.

According to the research literature, men tend to be more resilient when it comes to insufficient energy intake.

Based upon the rather limited research in this area, it seems that men are able to tolerate reduced energy intake before it affects us negatively. This indicates that women in particular are a vulnerable population in this respect, says Jeppesen.

The gender difference is partly due to the fact that low energy availability can cause a womans estrogen levels to drop drastically. Since estrogen protects the circulatory system, muscles and bones, etc., estrogen loss has extensive effects on a womans physiology.

Hellsten points out that theharmful effectsof not eating enough for long periods of time, especially in women, can therefore also be lifelong.

Author: Ylva Hellsten Source: University of Copenhagen Contact: Ylva Hellsten University of Copenhagen Image: The image is credited to Neuroscience News

Original Research: Open access. Low energy availability increases immune cell formation of reactive oxygen species and impairs exercise performance in female endurance athletes by Ylva Hellsten et al. Redox Biology

Abstract

Low energy availability increases immune cell formation of reactive oxygen species and impairs exercise performance in female endurance athletes

The effects of low energy availability (LEA) on the immune system are poorly understood. This study examined the effects of 14 days of LEA on immune cell redox balance and inflammation at rest and in response to acute exercise, and exercise performance in female athletes.

Twelve female endurance athletes (age: 26.83.4yrs, maximum oxygen uptake (O2max): 55.25.1 mLmin1kg1) were included in a randomized, single-blinded crossover study. They were allocated to begin with either 14 days of optimal energy availability diet (OEA, 522kcalkg fat free mass (FFM)1day1) or LEA diet (222kcalkg FFM1day1), followed by 3 days of refueling (OEA) with maintained training volume. Peripheral blood mononuclear cells (PBMCs) were isolated, and plasma obtained at rest before and after each dietary period. The PBMCs were used for analysis of mitochondrial respiration and H2O2emission and specific proteins. Exercise performance was assessed on cycle by a 20-min time trial and time to exhaustion at an intensity corresponding to 110%O2max).

LEA was associated with a 94% (P=0.003) increase in PBMC NADPH oxidase 2 protein content, and a 22% (P=0.013) increase in systemic cortisol. LEA also caused an alteration of several inflammatory related proteins (P<0.05). Acute exercise augmented H2O2emission in PBMCs (P<0.001) following both OEA and LEA, but to a greater extent following LEA. LEA also reduced the mobilization of white blood cells with acute exercise. After LEA, performance was reduced in both exercise tests (P<0.001), and the reduced time trial performance remained after the 3 days of refueling (P<0.001).

14 days of LEA in female athletes increased cortisol levels and had a pronounced effect on the immune system, including increased capacity for ROS production, altered plasma inflammatory proteome and lowered exercise induced mobilization of leukocytes. Furthermore, LEA resulted in a sustained impairment in exercise performance.

More here:
Low-Calorie Diets Harm Athletes Performance and Health - Neuroscience News

Reflective Thinking Boosts Teen Brain Resilience to Violence – Neuroscience News

Summary: Teens who engage in transcendent thinking can counteract the negative effects of violence exposure on brain development. This type of thinking involves considering broader ethical and societal implications of social issues.

The study showed that such reflective thinking leads to brain growth even in teens from high-violence communities. These findings emphasize the importance of fostering reflective thinking skills in adolescents.

Key Facts:

Source: USC

These latest findings from CANDLE (USC Center for Affective Neuroscience, Development, Learning and Education) researchers show that teens who think about social issues and violence in more reflective ways show greater resilience to the effects of violence exposure on their brain development.

The study was published in theJournal of Research on Adolescence.

Mary Helen Immordino-Yang and a team of CANDLE researchers have found that teens who engage in more transcendent thinking, that is thinking that moves beyond reacting to the specifics of social situations to also consider broader ethical, personal and societal implications, can counteract the negative impacts exposure to violence has on theirbrain development.

The study built onan earlier oneby Immordino-Yang that showed the disturbing link between adolescents exposure to violence in their community and their brain development.

In both studies, MRI brain scans of teens who grew up in communities with high levels of violenceshowed thinner cortex in parts of the brain that are involved in feeling stress and pain as well as motivation, judgment and emotional processing.

This new study confirms these links exist even in older teens, around age 1618 when they witness violence, but also offers a possible antidote. The 55 participants were all from low socioeconomic status backgrounds and lived in urban settings. The teens were asked about their exposure to community violence and underwent two MRI brain scans, one at the beginning of the study and one two years later.

At the time of the initial scans, participants also watched mini-documentaries about teens in compelling situations and discussed their reactions in a recorded interview which was later assessed for transcendent thinking.

The final MRI scans showed that the more a teen had engaged in transcendent thinking, the greater the brain growth in various areas across the two years, including those areas most impacted by the violence.

The findings suggest that teens transcendent thinking may be helping them to counteract the effect of exposure to violence on their brain development.

These findings reveal that as adolescents work to contextualize and make sense of the violence they are exposed to, this complex thinking builds resilience and thus grows their brains despite the violence they witness.

When the teens were able to reflect on such things as why violence happens and what can be done to get to the root of the problems, they showed a form of neural resilience in theiranterior cingulate cortex, among other regions.

Let me be clearwe found that witnessing community violence and crime, even in older teens, was associated with key regions of their brain losing volume over time. In effect, witnessing violence made regions of their brains shrink a bit, which is a pattern seen in people suffering from PTSD and in soldiers deployed to war, said Immordino-Yang.

At the same time, the kids were not passively being impactedwhen they showed us that they were thinking hard about why these things happen, and what could be done to make the world better for everyone involved, this kind of thinking grew their brain volume in these same brain regions. Violence was bad for them, but transcendent and civically oriented thinking was a kind of antidote, neurologically speaking.

The study builds on a body of research spearheaded by Immordino-Yang that investigates the effects of transcendent thinking on adolescent brain development. A recentlandmark studypublished by Immordino-Yang showed that transcendent thinking in adolescents can predict future brain growth and that this brain growth, in turn, predicts life satisfaction when youth transition to adulthood.

Immordino-Yangs teams findings underline the vulnerability of adolescents in communities impacted by high levels of violence while also emphasizing the importance of fostering skills like transcendent thinking in teens.

These skills cannot only helpteensmake sense of the violence they witness but also help them counteract the negative impact of thisviolenceon their developing brains.

Author: Kianoosh Hashemzadeh Source: USC Contact: Kianoosh Hashemzadeh USC Image: The image is credited to Neuroscience News

Original Research: Open access. Transcendent thinking counteracts longitudinal effects of midadolescent exposure to community violence in the anterior cingulate cortex by XiaoFei Yang et al. Journal of Research on Adolescence

Abstract

Transcendent thinking counteracts longitudinal effects of midadolescent exposure to community violence in the anterior cingulate cortex

Adolescence involves extensive brain maturation, characterized by social sensitivity and emotional lability, that co-occurs with increased independence. Mid-adolescence is also a hallmark developmental stage when youths become motivated to reflect on the broader personal, ethical, and systems-level implications of happenings, a process we term transcendent thinking.

Here, we examine the confluence of these developmental processes to ask, from a transdisciplinary perspective, how might community violence exposure (CVE) impact brain development during mid-adolescence, and how might youths dispositions for transcendent thinking be protective?

Fifty-five low-SES urban youth with no history of delinquency (32 female; 27 Latinx, 28 East Asian) reported their CVE and underwent structural MRI first at age 1418, and again 2years later.

At the studys start, participants also discussed their feelings about 40 minidocumentaries featuring other teens compelling situations in a 2-h private interview that was transcribed and coded for transcendent thinking.

Controlling for CVE and brain structure at the start: (1) New CVE during the 2-year inter-scan interval was associated with greater gray matter volume (GMV) reduction over that interval in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), a central network hub whose reduced volume has been associated with posttraumatic stress disorder, and across multiple additional cortical and subcortical regions; (2) participants transcendent thinking in the interview independently predicted greater GMV increase during the 2-year inter-scan interval in the ACC.

Findings highlight the continued vulnerability of mid-adolescents to community violence and the importance of supporting teens dispositions to reflect on the complex personal and systems-level implications and affordances of their civic landscape.

Go here to read the rest:
Reflective Thinking Boosts Teen Brain Resilience to Violence - Neuroscience News

What well-being is (and isnt), according to neuroscience – Big Think

Cultivating your own well-being does not mean getting rid of discomfort, according to neuroscientist Mary Helen Immordino-Yang.

Immordino-Yang is a professor of education, psychology, and neuroscience at the University of Southern California, and she has spent years researching what makes one well.

Turns out, true well-being comes from balance and flexibility, not just from filling your life with positive experiences. Immordino-Yang suggests a few practical tips for maintaining this balance, such as prioritizing quality relationships, monitoring our social media usage, and engaging in activities that bring joy and reflection.

We cant fully eradicate suffering, but we can accept it and choose to grow through it. By welcoming healthy levels of discomfort and taking agency over our own activities and habits, we can achieve wellness as it was meant to be achieved as a state of being, not a destination.

Mary Helen Immordino-Yang:Often we think about well-being as the absence of disease, the absence of mental illness, the absence of strife. But neuroscience and developmental social science help us understand that the origins of well-being are really about balance. It's about an ability and a flexibility to manage oneself. Well-being is both a capacity and a state.

The brain data really help us understand the contributions to that capacity and state. A concept like well-being is not applied to a person; it's conjured within the person by their own actions and dispositions of mind. This shifts the way in which we support a person in developing well-being and becoming well.

I think there are practical things that you can do to support your own well-being strategically. Prioritizing the quality of the relationships that you have with the people around you, whom you care about. Setting yourself up to have control over certain kinds of social media use, certain kinds of scrollingthese addictive things that suck you into a pattern of wanting more and pull you out of a space where you can reflect and just sort of be.

Construct meaningful stories about how that's happening and what that feels like. Privilege the things that you really enjoy doing with the people you really enjoy being with. Taking the time to reflect and think about what it's all for, and then enact that. Giving to others and being engaged with otherswe reap back the benefits of that.

Here is the original post:
What well-being is (and isnt), according to neuroscience - Big Think

Brain Areas Take Micro-Naps While the Rest Stays Awake – Neuroscience News

Summary: New research shows sleep can be detected by brain activity patterns just milliseconds long. This study found small brain regions can momentarily flicker awake or asleep, challenging traditional views on sleep and wake states.

Using advanced neural network analysis, researchers uncovered high-frequency patterns that define sleep. These findings could help study neurodevelopmental and neurodegenerative diseases linked to sleep disturbances.

Key Facts:

Source: UC Santa Cruz

Sleep and wake: theyre totally distinct states of being that define the boundaries of our daily lives. For years, scientists have measured the difference between these instinctual brain processes by observing brain waves, with sleep characteristically defined by slow, long-lasting waves measured in tenths of seconds that travel across the whole organ.

For the first time, scientists have found that sleep can be detected by patterns of neuronal activity just milliseconds long, 1000 times shorter than a second, revealing a new way to study and understand the basic brain wave patterns that govern consciousness.

They also show that small regions of the brain can momentarily flicker awake while the rest of the brain remains asleep, and vice versa from wake to sleep.

These findings, described ina new study published in the journalNature Neuroscience, are from a collaboration between the laboratories of Assistant Professor of Biology Keith Hengen at Washington University in St. Louis and Distinguished Professor of Biomolecular Engineering David Haussler at UC Santa Cruz. The research was carried out by Ph.D. students David Parks (UCSC) and Aidan Schneider (WashU).

Over four years of work, Parks and Schneider trained a neural network to study the patterns within massive amounts of brain wave data, uncovering patterns that occur at extremely high frequencies that have never been described before and challenge foundational, long-held conceptions of the neurological basis of sleep and wake.

With powerful tools and new computational methods, theres so much to be gained by challenging our most basic assumptions and revisiting the question of what is a state? Hengen said.

Sleep or wake is the single greatest determinant of your behavior, and then everything else falls out from there. So if we dont understand what sleep and wake actually are, it seems like weve missed the boat.

It was surprising to us as scientists to find that different parts of our brains actually take little naps when the rest of the brain is awake, although many people may have already suspected this in their spouse, so perhaps a lack of male-female bias is what is surprising, Haussler quipped.

Understanding sleep

Neuroscientists study the brain via recordings of the electrical signals of brain activity, known as electrophysiology data, observing voltage waves as they crest and fall at different paces. Mixed into these waves are the spike patterns of individual neurons.

The researchers worked with data from mice at the Hengen Lab in St. Louis. The freely-behaving animals were equipped with a very lightweight headset that recorded brain activity from 10 different brain regions for months at a time, tracking voltage from small groups of neurons with microsecond precision.

This much input created petabytes which are one million times larger than a gigabyte of data. David Parks led the effort to feed this raw data into an artificial neural network, which can find highly complex patterns, to differentiate sleep and wake data and find patterns that human observation may have missed.

A collaboration with theshared academic compute infrastructurelocated at UC San Diego enabled the team to work with this much data, which was on the scale of what large companies like Google or Facebook might use.

Knowing that sleep is traditionally defined by slow-moving waves, Parks began to feed smaller and smaller chunks of data into the neural network and asked it to predict if the brain was asleep or awake.

They found that the model could differentiate between sleep and wake from just milliseconds of brain activity data. This was shocking to the research team it showed that the model couldnt have been relying on the slow-moving waves to learn the difference between sleep and wake.

Just as listening to a thousandth of a second of a song couldnt tell you if it had a slow rhythm, it would be impossible for the model to learn a rhythm that occurs over several seconds by just looking at random isolated milliseconds of information.

Were seeing information at a level of detail thats unprecedented, Haussler said. The previous feeling was that nothing would be found there, that all the relevant information was in the slower frequency waves.

This paper says, if you ignore the conventional measurements, and you just look at the details of the high frequency measurement over just a thousandth of a second, there is enough there to tell if the tissue is asleep or not. This tells us that there is something going on a very fast scale thats a new hint to what might be going on in sleep.

Hengen, for his part, was convinced that Parks and Schneider had missed something, as their results were so contradictory to bedrock concepts drilled into him over many years of neuroscience education. He asked Parks to produce more and more evidence that this phenomena could be real.

This challenged me to ask myself to what extent are my beliefs based on evidence, and what evidence would I need to see to overturn those beliefs? Hengen said.

It really did feel like a game of cat and mouse, because Id ask David [Parks] over and over to produce more evidence and prove things to me, and hed come back and say check this out! It was a really interesting process as a scientist to have my students tear down these towers brick by brick, and for me to have to be okay with that.

Local patterns

Because an artificial neural network is fundamentally a black box and does not report back on what it learns from, Parks began stripping away layers of temporal and spatial information to try to understand what patterns the model could be learning from.

Eventually, they got down to the point where they were looking at chunks of brain data just a millisecond long and at the highest frequencies of brain voltage fluctuations.

Wed taken out all the information that neuroscience has used to understand, define, and analyze sleep for the last century, and we asked can the model still learn under these conditions? Parks said. This allowed us to look into signals we havent understood before.

By looking at these data, they were able to determine that the hyper-fast pattern of activity between just a few neurons was the fundamental element of sleep that the model was detecting. Crucially, such patterns cannot be explained by the traditional, slow and widespread waves.

The researchers hypothesize that the slow moving waves may be acting to coordinate the fast, local patterns of activity, but ultimately reached the conclusion that the fast patterns are much closer to the true essence of sleep.

If the slow moving waves traditionally used to define sleep are compared to thousands of people in a baseball stadium doing the wave, then these fast-moving patterns are the conversations between just a few people deciding to participate in the wave. Those conversations occurring are essential for the overall larger wave to take place, and are more directly related to the mood of the stadium the wave is a secondary result of that.

Observing flickers

In further studying the hyperlocal patterns of activity, the researchers began to notice another surprising phenomenon.

As they observed the model predicting sleep or wake, they noticed what looked at first like errors, in which for a split second the model would detect wake in one region of the brain while the rest of the brain remained asleep. They saw the same thing in wake states: for a split second, one region would fall asleep while the rest of the regions were awake. They call these instances flickers.

We could look at the individual time points when these neurons fired, and it was pretty clear that [the neurons] were transitioning to a different state, Schneider said. In some cases, these flickers might be constrained to the area of just an individual brain region, maybe even smaller than that.

This compelled the researchers to explore what flickers could mean about the function of sleep, and how they affect behavior during sleep and wake.

Theres a natural hypothesis there; lets say a small part of your brain slips into sleep while youre awake does that mean your behavior suddenly looks like youre asleep? We started to see that that was often the case, Schneider said.

In observing the behavior of mice, the researchers saw that when a brain region would flicker to sleep while the rest of the brain was awake, the mouse would pause for a second, almost like it had zoned out. A flicker during sleep (one brain region wakes up) was reflected by an animal twitching in its sleep.

Flickers are particularly surprising because they dont follow established rules dictating the strict cycle of the brain moving sequentially between wake to non-REM sleep to REM sleep.

We are seeing wake to REM flickers, REM to non-REM flickers we see all these possible combinations, and they break the rules that you would expect based on a hundred years of literature, Hengen said.

I think they reveal the separation between the macro-state sleep and wake at the level of the whole animal, and the fundamental unit of state in the brain the fast and local patterns.

Impact

Gaining a deeper understanding of the patterns that occur at high-frequencies and the flickers between wake and sleep could help researchers better study neurodevelopmental and neurodegenerative diseases, which are both associated with sleep dysregulation.

Both Haussler and Hengens lab groups are interested in understanding this connection further, with Haussler interested in further studying these phenomena in cerebral organoid models, bits of brain tissue grown on a laboratory bench.

This gives us potentially a very, very sharp scalpel with which to cut into these questions of diseases and disorders, Hengen said. The more we understand fundamentally about what sleep and wake are, the more we can address pertinent clinical and disease related problems.

On a foundational level, this work helps push forward our understanding of the many layers of complexity of the brain as the organ that dictates behavior, emotion, and much more.

Author: Emily Cerf Source: UC Santa Cruz Contact: Emily Cerf UC Santa Cruz Image: The image is credited to Neuroscience News

Original Research: Closed access. A nonoscillatory, millisecond-scale embedding of brain state provides insight into behavior by David Haussler et al. Nature Neuroscience

Abstract

A nonoscillatory, millisecond-scale embedding of brain state provides insight into behavior

The most robust and reliable signatures of brain states are enriched in rhythms between 0.1 and 20Hz. Here we address the possibility that the fundamental unit of brain state could be at the scale of milliseconds and micrometers.

By analyzing high-resolution neural activity recorded in ten mouse brain regions over 24h, we reveal that brain states are reliably identifiable (embedded) in fast, nonoscillatory activity.

Sleep and wake states could be classified from 100to 101ms of neuronal activity sampled from 100m of brain tissue. In contrast to canonical rhythms, this embedding persists above 1,000Hz.

This high-frequency embedding is robust to substates, sharp-wave ripples and cortical on/off states. Individual regions intermittently switched states independently of the rest of the brain, and such brief state discontinuities coincided with brief behavioral discontinuities.

Our results suggest that the fundamental unit of state in the brain is consistent with the spatial and temporal scale of neuronal computation.

See original here:
Brain Areas Take Micro-Naps While the Rest Stays Awake - Neuroscience News

Persistent protein pairing enables memories to last – The Transmitter: Neuroscience News and Perspectives

One question long plagued memory researcher Andr Fenton: How can memories last for years when a protein essential to maintaining them, called memory protein kinase Mzeta (PKMzeta), lasts for just days?

The answer, Fenton now says, may lie in PKMzetas interaction with another protein, called postsynaptic kidney and brain expressed adaptor protein (KIBRA). Complexes of the two molecules maintain memories in mice for at least one month, according to a new study co-led by Fenton, professor of neural science at New York University.

The bond between the two proteins protects each of them, Fenton says, from normal degradation in the cell.

KIBRA preferentially gloms onto potentiated synapses, the study shows. And it may help PKMzeta stick there, too, where the kinase acts as a molecular switch to help memories persist, Fenton says.

As Theseus Ship was sustained for generations by continually replacing worn planks with new timbers, long-term memory can be maintained by continual exchange of potentiating molecules at activated synapses, Fenton and his colleagues write in their paper, which was published last month in Science Advances.

Before this study, the PKMzeta mystery had two missing puzzle pieces, says Justin OHare, assistant professor of pharmacology at the University of Colorado Denver, who was not involved in the study.

One was how PKMzeta identifies potentiated synapses, part of the cellular mechanism underlying memory formation. The second was how memories persist despite the short lifetime of each PKMzeta molecule. This study essentially proposes KIBRA as a solution to both of thoseand the experiments themselves are pretty convincing and thorough. They do everything multiple ways.

P

The controversy, Fenton says, forced his team to look for another molecule that might be involved in long-term potentiation. They focused on KIBRA because the scaffolding protein is found in neurons and has been shown to interact with similar kinases in the sea slug.

In a common benchtop experiment of memory persistence, electrical stimulation of the CA3 region in mouse hippocampal slices induced complexes of KIBRA and PKMzeta to form in the synapses of the downstream CA1 stratum radiatum region, Fentons team found, confirming their suspicions.

Not only did [KIBRA and PKMzeta] interact, but they interacted in the right places for storing a memory or maintaining [long-term potentiation], Fenton says.

The excitatory postsynaptic potentials, a proxy for long-term potentiation, across CA1 neurons in the slices remained high three hours after stimulation, the researchers found, but then dipped back down to baseline after treatment with two moleculeszeta-stat and K-ZAPthat block the interaction between KIBRA and PKMzeta.

The two inhibitors also caused wildtype mice to forget their training in two different foot-shock experiments when administered either three days or one montha time frame longer than the kinases typical turnoverafter the animals had learned the task.

This result suggests that the KIBRA-PKMzeta complex is crucial for long-term potentiation and for memory maintenance, Fenton says.

E

That ruled out the possibility that other factorssuch as off-target effects of the two inhibitory moleculescaused the depotentiation or memory erasure in the mice. But it raised another question, Fenton says: So if PKMzeta is so important, and you delete it and you have normal memory and you have normal [long-term potentiation], like, what gives?

Another kinase, PKCiota/lambda, may step in and bind to KIBRA when PKMzeta is not around, Fenton says. Past work by Fenton and his colleagues has shown that PKCiota/lambda binds to KIBRA at a 10-fold lower rate than PKMzeta does.

This weaker interaction might explain why PKMzeta-null mice did maintain memories, but the memory is not as good, Fenton says. For example, in one experiment type, the PKMzeta-null mice re-enter an area where they previously received a mild foot shock more quickly than do their wildtype peers that did not receive the inhibitory molecules,but more slowly than the wildtype mice that received inhibitors,the study showed.

This result answers a question about another inhibitory molecule, the zeta inhibitor protein. ZIP, a 2020 study showed, interrupts long-term potentiation in mice that lack PKMzeta, indicating that memory relies on a completely different mechanism of action than PKMzeta, says Rami Yaka, professor of psychopharmacology at the Hebrew University of Jerusalem, who led the 2020 work but was not involved in the current study.

But ZIP is known to broadly target both PKMzeta and PKCiota/lambda, Fenton says. The inhibitors in the current study were specific to PKMzeta and so did not affect PKCiota/lambda.

There needed to be an explanation for why knocking out PKMzeta allowed memories to persist or [long-term potentiation] to persist, Fenton says.

How KIBRA becomes primed to capture PKMzeta and how KIBRA is attracted to the potentiated synapses remain open questions, says study investigator Panayiotis Tsokas, assistant professor in anesthesiology, physiology and pharmacology at SUNY Downstate Health Sciences University.

The answer might lie in calcium signaling in NMDA channels, which Tsokas says the team is exploring next.

Excerpt from:
Persistent protein pairing enables memories to last - The Transmitter: Neuroscience News and Perspectives

AI Enhances Story Creativity but Risks Reducing Novelty – Neuroscience News

Summary: A new study shows that AI helps make stories more creative, engaging, and well-written, especially for less creative writers. The research found that AI assistance boosts novelty and usefulness, making stories more enjoyable and less boring.

However, it also warns that the widespread use of AI may reduce the diversity and uniqueness of creative works. The findings highlight both the potential and risks of using AI in creative writing.

Key Facts:

Source: University of Exeter

Stories written with AI assistance have been deemed to be more creative, better written and more enjoyable.

A new study published in the journalScience Advancesfinds that AI enhances creativity by boosting the novelty of story ideas as well as the usefulness of stories their ability to engage the target audience and potential for publication.

It finds that AI professionalizes stories, making them more enjoyable, more likely to have plot twists, better written and less boring.

In a study in which 300 participants were tasked with writing a short, eight-sentence micro story for a target audience of young adults, researchers found that AI made those deemed less creative produce work that was up to 26.6% better written and 15.2% less boring.

However, AI was not judged to enhance the work produced by more creative writers.

The study also warns that while AI may enhance individual creativity it may also result in a loss of collective novelty, as AI-assisted stories were found to contain more similarities to each other and were less varied and diverse.

The researchers, from the University of Exeter Business School and Institute for Data Science and Artificial Intelligence as well as the UCL School of Management, assigned the 300 study participants to three groups: one group was allowed no AI help, a second group could use ChatGPT to provide a single three-sentence starting idea, and writers in the third group could choose from up to five AI-generated ideas for their inspiration.

They then recruited 600 people to judge how good the stories were, assessing them for novelty whether the stories did something new or unexpected and usefulness how appropriate they were for the target audience, and whether the ideas could be developed and potentially published.

They found that writers with the most access to AI experienced the greatest gains to their creativity, their stories scoring 8.1% higher for novelty and 9% higher for novelty compared with stories written without AI.

Writers who used up to five AI-generated ideas also scored higher for emotional characteristics, producing stories that were better written, more enjoyable, less boring and funnier.

The researchers evaluated the writers inherent creativity using a Divergent Association Task (DAT) and found that more creative writers those with the highest DAT scores benefitted least from generative AI ideas.

Less creative writers conversely saw a greater increase in creativity: access to five AI ideas improved novelty by 10.7% and usefulness by 11.5% compared with those who used no AI ideas. Their stories were judged to be up to 26.6% better written, up to 22.6%, more enjoyable and up to 15.2% less boring.

These improvements put writers with low DAT scores on a par with those with high DAT scores, effectively equalising creativity across the less and more creative writers.

The researchers also used OpenAIs embeddings application programming interface (API) to calculate how similar the stories were to each other.

They found a 10.7% increase in similarity between writers whose stories used one generative AI-idea, compared with the group that didnt use AI.

Oliver Hauser, Professor of Economics at the University of Exeter Business School and Deputy Director of the Institute for Data Science and Artificial Intelligence, said: This is a first step in studying a question fundamental to all human behaviour: how does generative AI affect human creativity?

Our results provide insight into how generative AI can enhance creativity, and removes any disadvantage or advantage based on the writers inherent creativity.

Anil Doshi, Assistant Professor at the UCL School of Management added: While these results point to an increase inindividualcreativity, there is risk of losingcollectivenovelty. Ifthe publishing industry were to embrace more generative AI-inspired stories, our findings suggest that the stories would become less unique in aggregate and moresimilar to each other.

Professor Hauser cautioned: This downward spiral shows parallels to an emerging social dilemma:if individual writers find out that their generative AI-inspired writing is evaluated as more creative,they have an incentive to use generative AI more in the future, but by doing so the collectivenovelty of stories may be reduced further.

In short, our results suggest that despite theenhancement effect that generative AI had on individual creativity, there may be a cautionary noteif generative AI were adopted more widely for creative tasks.

Author: Louise Vennells Source: University of Exeter Contact: Louise Vennells University of Exeter Image: The image is credited to Neuroscience News

Original Research: Open access. AI found to boost individual creativity at the expense of less varied content by Oliver Hauser et al. Science Advances

Abstract

AI found to boost individual creativity at the expense of less varied content

Creativity is core to being human. Generative artificial intelligence (AI)including powerful large language models (LLMs)holds promise for humans to be more creative by offering new ideas, or less creative by anchoring on generative AI ideas.

We study the causal impact of generative AI ideas on the production of short stories in an online experiment where some writers obtained story ideas from an LLM. We find that access to generative AI ideas causes stories to be evaluated as more creative, better written, and more enjoyable, especially among less creative writers.

However, generative AIenabled stories are more similar to each other than stories by humans alone. These results point to an increase in individual creativity at the risk of losing collective novelty. This dynamic resembles a social dilemma: With generative AI, writers are individually better off, but collectively a narrower scope of novel content is produced.

Our results have implications for researchers, policy-makers, and practitioners interested in bolstering creativity.

Read more here:
AI Enhances Story Creativity but Risks Reducing Novelty - Neuroscience News

Infection Brain Inflammation Triggers Muscle Weakness – Neuroscience News

Summary: A new study reveals how brain inflammation from infections and neurodegenerative diseases causes muscle weakness by releasing the IL-6 protein. Researchers found that IL-6 travels from the brain to muscles, reducing their energy production and function.

This discovery could lead to treatments for muscle wasting in diseases like Alzheimers and long COVID. Blocking the IL-6 pathway may prevent muscle weakness associated with brain inflammation.

Key Facts:

Source: WUSTL

Infections and neurodegenerative diseases cause inflammation in the brain. But for unknown reasons, patients with brain inflammation often develop muscle problems that seem to be independent of the central nervous system.

Now, researchers at Washington University School of Medicine in St. Louis have revealed how brain inflammation releases a specific protein that travels from the brain to the muscles and causes a loss of muscle function.

The study, in fruit flies and mice, also identified ways to block this process, which could have implications for treating or preventing the muscle wasting sometimes associated with inflammatory diseases, including bacterial infections, Alzheimers disease and long COVID.

The study is published July 12 in the journalScience Immunology.

We are interested in understanding the very deep muscle fatigue that is associated with some common illnesses, said senior authorAaron Johnson, PhD, an associate professor of developmental biology.

Our study suggests that when we get sick, messenger proteins from the brain travel through the bloodstream and reduce energy levels in skeletal muscle. This is more than a lack of motivation to move because we dont feel well. These processes reduce energy levels in skeletal muscle, decreasing the capacity to move and function normally.

To investigate the effects of brain inflammation on muscle function, the researchers modeled three different types of diseases anE. colibacterial infection, a SARS-CoV-2 viral infection and Alzheimers. When the brain is exposed to inflammatory proteins characteristic of these diseases, damaging chemicals called reactive oxygen species build up.

The reactive oxygen species cause brain cells to produce an immune-related molecule called interleukin-6 (IL-6), which travels throughout the body via the bloodstream. The researchers found that IL-6 in mice and the corresponding protein in fruit flies reduced energy production in muscles mitochondria, the energy factories of cells.

Flies and mice that had COVID-associated proteins in the brain showed reduced motor function the flies didnt climb as well as they should have, and the mice didnt run as well or as much as control mice, Johnson said.

We saw similar effects on muscle function when the brain was exposed to bacterial-associated proteins and the Alzheimers protein amyloid beta. We also see evidence that this effect can become chronic. Even if an infection is cleared quickly, the reduced muscle performance remains many days longer in our experiments.

Johnson, along with collaborators at the University of Florida and first author Shuo Yang, PhD who did this work as a postdoctoral researcher in Johnsons lab make the case that the same processes are likely relevant in people. The bacterial brain infection meningitis is known to increase IL-6 levels and can be associated with muscle issues in some patients, for instance.

Among COVID-19 patients, inflammatory SARS-CoV-2 proteins have been found in the brain during autopsy, and many long COVID patients report extreme fatigue and muscle weakness even long after the initial infection has cleared. Patients with Alzheimers disease also show increased levels of IL-6 in the blood as well as muscle weakness.

The study pinpoints potential targets for preventing or treating muscle weakness related to brain inflammation. The researchers found that IL-6 activates what is called the JAK-STAT pathway in muscle, and this is what causes the reduced energy production of mitochondria.

Several therapeutics already approved by the Food and Drug Administration for other diseases can block this pathway. JAK inhibitors as well as several monoclonal antibodies against IL-6 are approved to treat various types of arthritis and manage other inflammatory conditions.

Were not sure why the brain produces a protein signal that is so damaging to muscle function across so many different disease categories, Johnson said.

If we want to speculate about possible reasons this process has stayed with us over the course of human evolution, despite the damage it does, it could be a way for the brain to reallocate resources to itself as it fights off disease. We need more research to better understand this process and its consequences throughout the body.

In the meantime, we hope our study encourages more clinical research into this pathway and whether existing treatments that block various parts of it can help the many patients who experience this type of debilitating muscle fatigue, he said.

Yang S, Tian M, Dai Y, Wang R, Yamada S, Feng S, Wang Y, Chhangani D, Ou T, Li W, Guo X, McAdow J, Rincon-Limas DE, Yin X, Tai W, Cheng G, Johnson A. Infection and chronic disease activate a systemic brain-muscle signaling axis that regulates muscle function.Science Immunology. July 12, 2024.

Funding: This work is supported by the National Institutes of Health (NIH), grant numbers R01 AR070299 and R01AG059871; the National Key Research and Development Plan of China, grant numbers 2021YFC2302405, 2021YFC2300200, 2022YFC2303200, 2022YFC2303400 and 2022YFE0140700; the National Natural Science Foundation of China, grant numbers 32188101, 82271872, 32100755, 32172940 and 82341046; the Shenzhen San-Ming Project for Prevention and Research on Vector-borne Diseases, grant number SZSM202211023; the Yunnan Provincial Science and Technology Project at Southwest United Graduate School, grant number 202302AO370010; the New Cornerstone Science Foundation through the New Cornerstone Investigator Program; the Xplorer Prize from Tencent Foundation; the Natural Science Foundation of Heilongjiang Province, grant number JQ2021C005; the Science Fund Program for Distinguished Young Scholars (Overseas); and the Shenzhen Bay Laboratory Startup Fund, grant number 2133011.

Author: Jessica Church Source: WUSTL Contact: Jessica Church WUSTL Image: The image is credited to Neuroscience News

Original Research: Closed access. Infection and chronic disease activate a systemic brain-muscle signaling axis by Aaron Johnson et al. Science Immunology

Abstract

Infection and chronic disease activate a systemic brain-muscle signaling axis

Infections and neurodegenerative diseases induce neuroinflammation, but affected individuals often show nonneural symptoms including muscle pain and muscle fatigue. The molecular pathways by which neuroinflammation causes pathologies outside the central nervous system (CNS) are poorly understood.

We developed multiple models to investigate the impact of CNS stressors on motor function and found thatEscherichia coliinfections and SARS-CoV-2 protein expression caused reactive oxygen species (ROS) to accumulate in the brain. ROS induced expression of the cytokine Unpaired 3 (Upd3) inDrosophilaand its ortholog, IL-6, in mice.

CNS-derived Upd3/IL-6 activated the JAK-STAT pathway in skeletal muscle, which caused muscle mitochondrial dysfunction and impaired motor function. We observed similar phenotypes after expressing toxic amyloid- (A42) in the CNS.

Infection and chronic disease therefore activate a systemic brain-muscle signaling axis in which CNS-derived cytokines bypass the connectome and directly regulate muscle physiology, highlighting IL-6 as a therapeutic target to treat disease-associated muscle dysfunction.

See the article here:
Infection Brain Inflammation Triggers Muscle Weakness - Neuroscience News

Alto Neuroscience, Inc. (NYSE:ANRO) Receives Average Rating of Buy from Analysts – Defense World

Alto Neuroscience, Inc. (NYSE:ANRO Get Free Report) has been given a consensus recommendation of Buy by the six research firms that are currently covering the stock, Marketbeat Ratings reports. Six equities research analysts have rated the stock with a buy rating. The average 1 year price target among brokers that have issued a report on the stock in the last year is $35.00.

Several equities research analysts have weighed in on the stock. Rodman & Renshaw assumed coverage on shares of Alto Neuroscience in a research report on Friday, June 21st. They issued a buy rating and a $43.00 price objective for the company. Stifel Nicolaus restated a buy rating and issued a $32.00 price objective on shares of Alto Neuroscience in a research report on Monday, March 25th. Finally, William Blair restated an outperform rating on shares of Alto Neuroscience in a research report on Wednesday, June 12th.

Get Our Latest Stock Report on Alto Neuroscience

Large investors have recently modified their holdings of the stock. University of Texas Texas AM Investment Managment Co. bought a new position in Alto Neuroscience during the first quarter worth about $340,000. Zimmer Partners LP bought a new position in Alto Neuroscience during the first quarter worth about $1,151,000. AWM Investment Company Inc. bought a new position in Alto Neuroscience during the first quarter worth about $4,592,000. Artal Group S.A. bought a new position in Alto Neuroscience during the first quarter worth about $5,372,000. Finally, Jennison Associates LLC bought a new position in Alto Neuroscience during the first quarter worth about $7,039,000.

NYSE ANRO opened at $14.50 on Tuesday. The company has a quick ratio of 26.02, a current ratio of 26.02 and a debt-to-equity ratio of 0.05. The businesss fifty day moving average price is $12.21. Alto Neuroscience has a 1 year low of $9.40 and a 1 year high of $24.00.

Alto Neuroscience (NYSE:ANRO Get Free Report) last issued its quarterly earnings results on Tuesday, May 14th. The company reported ($0.76) earnings per share for the quarter, missing analysts consensus estimates of ($0.46) by ($0.30). On average, sell-side analysts expect that Alto Neuroscience will post -2.93 earnings per share for the current fiscal year.

(Get Free Report

Alto Neuroscience, Inc operates as a clinical-stage biopharmaceutical company in the United States. Its product pipeline comprising ALTO-100, which is in phase 2b clinical trial for the treatment of patients with major depressive disorder (MDD); and which is in phase 2a clinical trial for the treatment of post-traumatic stress disorder.

Receive News & Ratings for Alto Neuroscience Daily - Enter your email address below to receive a concise daily summary of the latest news and analysts' ratings for Alto Neuroscience and related companies with MarketBeat.com's FREE daily email newsletter.

Here is the original post:
Alto Neuroscience, Inc. (NYSE:ANRO) Receives Average Rating of Buy from Analysts - Defense World

2024 Kavli Prize awarded for research on face-selective brain areas – The Transmitter: Neuroscience News and Perspectives

Three pioneers in face-perception research have won the 2024 Kavli Prize in Neuroscience.

Nancy Kanwisher, professor of cognitive neuroscience at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology; Winrich Freiwald, professor of neurosciences and behavior at Rockefeller University; and Doris Tsao, professor of neurobiology at the University of California, Berkeley, will share the $1 million Kavli Prize for their discoveries of the regionsin both the human and monkey brainsresponsible for identifying and recognizing faces.

This is work thats very classic and very elegant, not only in face-processing and face-recognition work, but the impact its had on how we think about brain organization in general is huge, says Alexander Cohen, assistant professor of neurology at Harvard Medical School, who studies face recognition in autistic people.

The Norwegian Academy of Science and Letters awards the prize every two years.

K

To get to the root of face processing, Kanwisher spent hours as a young researcher lying still in an MRI machine as images of faces and objects flashed before her. A spot in the bottom right of the cerebral cortex lit up when she and others looked at faces, according to functional MRI (fMRI) scans, she and her colleagues reported in a seminal 1997 paper. They called the region the fusiform face area.

This discovery offered some of the first concrete evidence that the brain specializes in sections, rather than working as a giant, adaptable generalist, Kanwisher says. This shows that for some mental functions, theres a very particular part of the brain that does just that and only that thing.

The discovery revolutionized how we thought about specialization of the brain, Cohen says.

Two other face-sensitive regionsthe occipital and superior temporal sulcus face areasprocess parts of the face, such as the eyes, nose and mouth, and changeable aspects, such as gaze direction, subsequent work showed.

But knowing that regions of the human brain selectively respond to a face cannot tell a researcher much about how or why this happens, Kanwisher says. Tsao and Freiwald built on Kanwishers findings by carrying out studies in macaque monkeys to answer questions that studies in people could not. They used fMRI to scan 10 of the animals while showing them pictures of human faces, macaque faces, hands, gadgets, fruits and vegetables, headless bodies and scrambled patterns.

The monkeys brains have six distinct face patches, thought to be analogous to the areas seen in people, Tsao and Freiwald reported in a 2008 study.

Individual cells in these face patch regions specialize in recognizing faces seen from different angleslooking up, down, tilted to the side, and in profile, for instanceaccording to electrophysiological recordings, suggesting these specialized modules work together across regions, the team discovered.

Specific neurons can even recognize the different components that go into forming a facefrom hair to pupils, Tsao and Freiwald found in additional work involving electrode recordings.

Thats when we got this picture that the face patches are really like this assembly line that are building this invariant representation of facial identity, Tsao says.

Two additional brain areas in macaques temporal lobe specifically respond to familiar faces and not unfamiliar ones, Freiwald and his colleagues later identified using fMRI.

C

Tsao echoes her enthusiasm for the launchpad these findings have offered for future brain mapping. When we first started working on the face-patch system, people said its a total unicorn, Tsao says. That turned out to be completely wrong. It turns out that the face-patch system basically is a Rosetta Stone for all of the IT [inferior temporal] cortex. All of the IT cortex is organized in exactly the same way.

Understanding how we see faces can also be a tool for understanding more complex mental processes, such as memory and emotions, that are linked with social interactions, Freiwald says. Faces are the social stimulus for visual and social animals like us.

See the original post:
2024 Kavli Prize awarded for research on face-selective brain areas - The Transmitter: Neuroscience News and Perspectives